Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity

Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity
Java Games: Flashcards, matching, concentration, and word search.
Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity
Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity

Tools

  • Copy this to my account
  • E-mail to a friend
  • Find other activities
  • Start over
  • Help

AB
influence resulting from a person’s desire to gain approval or avoid disapproval normative social influence
the theory that we act to reduce the discomfort we feel when two thoughts are inconsistent. For example, when our awareness of our attitudes and of our actions clash, we can reduce the resulting dissonance by changing our attitudes 2. cognitive dissonance theory
the tendency for people who have first agreed to a small request to comply later with a larger request 3. foot-in-the-door phenomenon
the mode of thinking that occurs when the desire for harmony in a decision-making group overrides a realistic appraisal of alternatives groupthink
feelings, often based on our beliefs, that predispose us to respond in a particular way to objects, people, and events attitude
the tendency for observers, when analyzing another’s behavior to underestimate the impact of the situation and to overestimate the impact of personal disposition 6. fundamental attribution error
the loss of self-awareness and self-restraint occurring in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity deindividuation
suggests how we explain someone’s behavior—by crediting either the situation or the person’s disposition attribution theory
the tendency for people in a group to exert less effort when pooling their efforts toward attaining a common goal than when individually accountable social loafing
the scientific study of how we think about, influence, and relate to one another social psychology
stronger responses on simple or well-learned tasks in the presence of others social facilitation
influence resulting from one’s willingness to accept others’ opinions about reality informational social influence
the enhancement of a group’s prevailing inclinations through discussion within the group group polarization
adjusting one’s behavior or thinking to coincide with a group standard conformity

Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity

Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity

Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity

Mr. Fravel

Government Instructor

Highland Springs High School; Social Studies Department

Highland Springs, VA
  • View profile
  • Send e-mail

This activity was created by a Quia Web subscriber.
Learn more about Quia
Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity
Create your own activities

  1. Social Psychology
  2. Deindividuation

By Charlotte Nickerson, published Oct 08, 2021

Definition

  • Deindividuation is a state when you become so immersed in the norms of the group that you lose your sense of identity and personal responsibility. An individual relinquishes individual responsibility for actions and sees behavior as a consequence of group norms and expectations.
  • Le Bon was the first to recognize how individuals’ behavior changed in a crowd.
  • Anonymity leads to loss of internal restraints, self-identity and responsibility for our behavior. Conditions that increase anonymity serve to minimize concerns about evaluation by others and weaken normal controls based on guilt, shame or fear.
  • Diffusion of responsibility leads to less feelings of personal guilt when committing aggressive acts.
  • Prentice-Dunn et al. (1982) suggested it was a reduced self awareness rather than simply anonymity that leads to de-individuation. They proposed two types of self-awareness; public, and private.

Definition and Historical Background

Deindividuation is a process where people lose their sense of socialized individual identity and resort to unsocialized and anti-social behavior.

Deindividuation is a state of decreased self-evaluation in a crowd, and is one of the most widely-cited effects of social groups (Postmes and Spears, 1998).

Deindividuation is characterized by a lower awareness of the self and one’s individuality. This may result in anti-normative and disinhibited behavior; “what forces crowd members at times to behave in uncivilized and violent ways (Diener, 1976).

Classical Theories Gustave Le Bon’s crowd theory provides the basis for deindividuation (Postmes, 2001). In The Crowd, Le Bon proposes that anonymity, suggestibility, and contagion can turn groups of people into so-called “psychological crowds.”

The collective mind of the crowd takes that of the individual, and individual crowd members become “irrational, fickle, and suggestible” (Postmes, 2001), and as a result becomes controlled by the crowd’s leader, thus capable of performing anti-normative acts.

Le Bon’s theory of The Crowd came to influence many theories of collective behavior, such as those of Freud, McDougall, Blumer, and Allport. The 1950s brought a resurgence in popularity for Le Bon’s idea.

Festinger et al. (1952) put Le Bon’s theory of the crowd into scientific terms, describing deindividuation as a state where individuals are not, “seen or paid attention to as individuals,” and for which, “under conditions where the member is not individuated in the group, there is likely to occur of the member a reduction of inner restraints against doing various things'.

Early theorists, such as Festinger (1952), believed that deindividuation occurred when those in a group are not given individual treatment; individuals are unaccountable.

This unaccountability leads to a reduction in inner restraint, increasing behavior that would normally be inhibited. Like Lee Bon, Festinger defined deindividuation as a loss of individuality through submergence in a crowd.

However, to Festinger, this individuality is not replaced by the collective mind. Rather, deindividuation simply releases an individual from their normal moral restraints (Postmes and Spears, 1998).


Philip Zimbardo's Approach to Deindividuation

Zimbardo did not see deindividuation as solely group phenomenon, applying deindividualization as broadly as suicide, murder, and hostility in relationships (Postmes and Spears, 1998).

Although he studied anti-social behavior, Zimbardo stressed that deindividuated acts can be prosocial. When individuals had minimal self-observation, self-evaluation, and concern for social evaluation, Zimbardo argued, individuals have a weakening of controls based on guilt, shame, fear, and commitment (Zimbardo, 1969).

Zimbardo (1969) conducted an influential study which inspired the field of deindividuation research as a whole. Researchers clothed one group of participants anonymously in oversized lab coats and hoods, and a control group with typical clothes and name tags.

The participants were assigned a task similar to that of the Milgram obedience experiment — to shock a so-called participant (an actor) so long as the researchers demanded that they do so.

Those clothed in the anonymizing white robes shocked these actors for a longer amount of time than individuals in typical clothing (Postmes, 2001). However, Zimbardo reported that a replication of his study — the participants dressed as non-anonymous soldiers this time obtained the opposite results.

Although psychologists widely believed Zimbardo’s methodology to have empirical problems, other experiments conducted nearly identical experiments (Postmes, 1998).

Psychologists such as Edward Diener soon devised less contrived contexts to test the validity of Zimbardo’s findings. Diener believed that Zimbardo and other researchers showed inconsistent support for the deindividuation hypothesis because of a lack of concern with the individual psychological changes of deindividuation (Diener, 1980).

For example, in one study by Diener, Westford, Dineen, and Fraser (1973), participants were required to hit a so-called pacifist (an actor trained to be non-responsive). If deindividuation held, researchers believed, then there would be greater aggression toward the pacifist in crowds.

However, results of these studies were inconsistent, or even contradictory. In one study, for example, isolated individuals actually displayed more aggression than groups (Postmes and Spears, 1998).

Diener et. al. also tested the deindividuation hypothesis in the context of trick-or-treating through a field study. Diener et. al. aimed to measure anti-normative behavior (stealing candy) as a function of anonymity and group size). However, the field setting made it virtually impossible to measure the self-awareness of participants.

In Prentice-Dunn and Rogers (1982), individuals were instructed to focus attention outward through being seated in a dimly lit room with loud music playing, where participants played video games and were encouraged to speak among each other.

Those in an inward-facing group played less arousing games in a well-lit quiet room. In the former condition, consistent with deindividuation theory, participants had higher levels of aggression (Postmes, 2001).


Criticism and Reconceptualization

Historically, evaluating the research relating to deindividuation has been difficult, as the definition of deindividuation has evolved rapidly. Researchers have seen deindividuation theory as neglecting how crowd behavior may be the product of local group norms.

As late as the 1970s, deindividuation was a focus of psychological research, but there remained little empirical support for deindividuation theory. The evidence supporting deindividuation driven by anonymity was inconsistent and there remained no evidence for the state of deindividuation itself (Diener, 1980).

As a result, the next decade saw a reconceptualization of deindividuation theory. The main contributing factor in anti-normative behavior in deindividuated groups shifted from a lack of accountability among group members to the anonymity that belonging to the group brings.

According to Le Boon, collective behavior is always irrational and individuals in the crowd lose their intellectual control. However, sociologists beyond Le Bon found that crowds can actually encourage norms for restraint and orderly behavior (Turner and Killian, 1972).

Theorists postulated that group behavior was a consequence of local group norms rather than the anonymization of individuals within groups. Johnson and Downing 1979 tested this hypothesis by recreating Zimbardo’s original experiment and dressing participants in either uniforms similar to that of the Ku Klux Klan or nurses.

Those dressed as Ku Klux Klan members shocked actors more, and those dressed as nurses shocked actors less than the control group. Thus, this research supports the ideas that those in groups are sensitive to, and highly influenced by, normative cues.

Prentice-Dunn et al. (1982) suggested it was a reduced self awareness rather than simply anonymity that leads to de-individuation. They proposed two types of self-awareness.
  • Public self-awareness The individual’s concern about the impression presented to other people, knowing that they will be evaluated. This is reduced by being in a crowd, diffusion of responsibility, anonymity and role models within the group which set the social norms. Loss of public awareness leads to loss of public standards of behavior or a lowering of inhibitions. If an individual is self-focused, they act according to their own values so aggressive behavior is less likely.
  • Private self- awareness Concern individuals have for their own thoughts and feelings. This is reduced by becoming so involved with group activities that individuals “forget” themselves. Loss of private awareness leads to a loss of internal standards and an over-reliance on environmental cues. If an individual is focused on a group, their private self-awareness is decreased and they are less able to monitor their own behavior so aggressive behavior is more likely.

Postmes and Spears’ 1998 review, Deindividuation and antinormative behavior, a meta analysis, showed little support for the occurrence of deindividuated, antinormative behavior or the existence of a deindividuated state.

However, this analysis went on to show that groups and individuals conform more to norms specific to a situation while “deindividuated.” Prentice-Dunn and Rogers (1982) attempted to account for the inconsistencies in deindividuation theory by extending the theory further, into “differential self-awareness theory.”

According to differential self-awareness theory, there are two types of cues that disinhibit collective behavior. “Accountability cues,” such as anonymity and diffusion of responsibility decrease public self awareness, resulting in crowd members less concerned with self-evaluation and not expecting negative consequences for their behavior.

Conversely, “attentional cues,” such as group cohesiveness and physiological arousal draw attention away from oneself and one’s own behavior, resulting in decreased self-regulation and attention to personal standards for behavior.

Although both accountability cues and attentional cues can result in antinormative, disinhibited behavior, only reduced private self awareness could lead to deindividuated behavior, as defined by Prentice-Dunn and Rogers (1989).


Social Identity Theory's Explanation of Deindividuation

The definition and usage of deindividuation has shifted markedly over time. Early deindividuation theorists such as Frestinger et al. (1952) define deindividuation as associated with not being scrutinized or accountable when in a group.

Zimbardo (1969) later attempted to define the inputs leading to and subsequent effects of deindividuation.

Zimbardo (1969) extended Festinger’s theory of deindividuation to create a theoretical framework specifying the variables leading to deindividuation and output behavior. The factors leading to deindividuation, in the 1960s, expanded from solely anonymity to contextual factors such as reductions of responsibility, arousal, sensory overload, a lack of predictability, and the effects of drugs and alcohol (Zimbardo, 1969). The behavior created by these factors are “in violation of established norms of appropriateness.” In particular, Zimbardo characterized deindividuated behavior as emotional, impulsive, irrational, regressive, and intense.

This deindividuation involved reduced feelings of self-observation, but accountability to an audience. And finally, Diener saw reduced self-awareness as being the defining feature of deindividuation. Diener ultimately expanded deindividuation theory but kept it within the realm of classical deindividuation theory.

In contrast, Prentice-Dunn and Rogers (1982) eliminated the anonymity that results in a lack of conscious accountability resulting in a fear of negative consequences. Social identity theorists argue that a deindividuation situation does not cause a loss of self, but a shift in identity from individual to member of a crowd.

For example, the collective identity of a group of protestors may prove to unite demonstrators who otherwise have strong divisions between them; and, conversely, the entire group may revolt if another group (e.g. police officers) attempts to act on the entire group as if it were one (Reicher et. al., 1995).

The most dominant social identity theory of deindividuation is the social identity model of deindividuation effects (SIDE). The SIDE model argues that the factors that lead to deindividuation, such as anonymity, group cohesiveness, and group immersion can reinforce group salience and conformity to group norms (Postmes and Spears, 1998).

Furthermore, the SIDE model predicts conformity to the social norms of a specific group or social identity rather than conformity to general norms. The SIDE model can explain the outcomes of several deindividuation experiments.

For example, the SIDE model may attribute the more aggressive behavior while playing video games in a dark room with loud rock music as a consequence of male college student participants believed that aggression was expected and consistent with their group identity in that situation (Postmes and Spears, 1998), and placing participants in front of, for example, a mirror, could focus attention on individual identity (Froming, Walker, and Lopyan, 1982; Postmes and Spears, 1998).

Being aware of one’s individuality can also moderate how participants see each others’ norms. For example, in Froming et. al. (1982) — the experiment where participants shocked actors — participants low in private self-awareness shocked more when they believed other people favored shocking more and less when they believed other people shocked less.


Examples

Deindividuation, at its extremes, is responsible for the collective behavior of violent crowds, such as lynch mobs, as well as mass sociaal phenomena such as genocide, stereotyping, and disinhibited behavior online.

Collective Responses to Health Crises Lewis, Himmelberger, and Elmore (2020) examined the effects of self-reflection on pro-social behavior, citing the COVID-19 pandemic as a possible application.

Wearing surgical masks, according to Mullen et. al. (2003) decreases self-awareness and increases the likelihood of selfish behavior (Miller and Rowold, 1979), and the increasing anonymized interactions of those during the pandemic may have led to deindividuation.

Genocide

Researchers such as Staub (1989) believe that deindividuation is a necessary factor leading to genocide. Hitler and Mouselllini both studied the likes of Le Bon’s crowd theory in creating their programs of mass-internment and murder (Postmes and Spears, 1998).

By encouraging deindividuation among members of a group, these individuals become less self-aware, less responsible for their actions, and therefore more likely to engage in violence.

Authoritarian governments often create organizations and military groups designed to enhance deindividuation. For example, uniforms and clearly prescribed rules for behavior can facilitate individuals committing acts of violence by enhancing deindividuation through conformity and diffusion of responsibility (Woolf and Hulsizer, 2007).

Such examples of groups promoting deindividuation through these means were Hitler’s youth and the Khmer Rouge. Such groups, such as those of the SS, required and inculcated an extreme willingness to endure danger and submit to authority, demanding fighting and killing from the start. The shared training, experience, and privileges of those in these authoritarian organizations promoted strong group ties, and ordinary rules and prohibitions did not apply to members of the SS legally.

As a result, the SS encouraged deindividuation and subsequently collective actions that broke down moral prohibitions to mass murder (Staub, 1989).

The Internet

There have been a number of studies examining the impacts of deindividuation on anti-normative behavior over computer-mediated communication, such as the internet.

In particular, Lee (2007) studied how deindividuation affects group polarization while communicating via a computer. Participants either shared personal information (individuated) or remain completely anonymous (deindividuated) before sharing options on several social problems.

Those who were deindividuated tended to identify more strongly with their partner, and defended their arguments more strongly when compared with “public” evaluations, which is consistent with the SIDE model of deindividuation.

Deindividuation on the internet is also associated with individuals committing illegal acts.

For example, Hinduja (2008) found that those who experience and prefer anonymity and pseudonymity while using the internet were more likely to pirate software than others.

Reducing Deindividualization

Those who have greater levels of self-awareness show lower rates of anti-normative behavior because of deindividuation. This self-awareness has often been simulated by mirrors (Postmes and Spears 1998).

For example, students who take tests in front of mirrors cheat at lower rates than those who do not (Diener and Wallbom, 1976).

About the Author

Charlotte Nickerson is a member of the Class of 2024 at Harvard University. Coming from a research background in biology and archeology, Charlotte currently studies how digital and physical space shapes human beliefs, norms, and behaviors and how this can be used to create businesses with greater social impact.

How to reference this article:

How to reference this article:

Nickerson, C. (2021, Oct 09). What is deindividuation in psychology? definition and examples. Simply Psychology. www.simplypsychology.org/what-is-deindividuation.html

References

Diener, E., Fraser, S. C., Beaman, A. L., & Kelem, R. T. (1976). Effects of deindividuation variables on stealing among Halloween trick-or-treaters. Journal of personality and social psychology, 33(2), 178.

Diener, E., Lusk, R., DeFour, D., & Flax, R. (1980). Deindividuation: Effects of group size, density, number of observers, and group member similarity on self-consciousness and disinhibited behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology, 39(3), 449.

Diener, E., & Wallbom, M. (1976). Effects of self-awareness on antinormative behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 10(1), 107-111.

Diener, E., & Wallbom, M. (1976). Effects of self-awareness on antinormative behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 10(1), 107-111.

Diener, E., Westford, K. L., Dineen, J., & Fraser, S. C. (1973). Beat the pacifist: The deindividuating effects of anonymity and group presence. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association.

Froming, W. J., Walker, G. R., & Lopyan, K. J. (1982). Public and private self-awareness: When personal attitudes conflict with societal expectations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18(5), 476-487.

Hinduja, S. (2008). Deindividuation and internet software piracy. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 11(4), 391-398.

Lee, E.-J. (2007). Deindividuation effects on group polarization in computer-mediated communication: The role of group identification, public-self-awareness, and perceived argument quality. Journal of communication, 57(2), 385-403.

Lewis, J. A., Himmelberger, Z. M., & Elmore, J. D. (2020). I can see myself helping: The effect of self‐awareness on prosocial behavior. International Journal of Psychology.

Miller, F. G., & Rowold, K. L. (1979). Halloween masks and deindividuation. Psychological Reports.

Mullen, B., Migdal, M. J., & Rozell, D. (2003). Self-awareness, deindividuation, and social identity: Unraveling theoretical paradoxes by filling empirical lacunae. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(9), 1071-1081.

Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (1998). Deindividuation and antinormative behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 123(3), 238.

Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (1998). Deindividuation and antinormative behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 123(3), 238.

Prentice-Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. (1982). Effects of public and private self-awareness on deindividuation and aggression. Journal of personality and social psychology, 43(3), 503.

Reicher, S. D., Spears, R., & Postmes, T. (1995). A social identity model of deindividuation phenomena. European review of social psychology, 6(1), 161-198.

Smelser, N. J., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (Vol. 11): Elsevier Amsterdam.

Staub, E. (1989). The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other group violence: Cambridge University Press.

Turner, R., & Killian, L. (1972). Social Movements: Character and Processes. Collective Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Woolf, L. M., & Hulsizer, M. R. (2007). Understanding the mosaic of humanity through research methodology: Infusing diversity into research methods courses. In Best practices for teaching statistics and research methods in the behavioral sciences (pp. 237-256): Routledge.

Zimbardo, P. G. (1969). The human choice: Individuation, reason, and order versus deindividuation, impulse, and chaos. Paper presented at the Nebraska symposium on motivation.

Further Information

Reicher, S. D., Spears, R., & Postmes, T. (1995). A social identity model of deindividuation phenomena. European review of social psychology, 6(1), 161-198. Vilanova, F., Beria, F. M., Costa, Â. B., & Koller, S. H. (2017). Deindividuation: From Le Bon to the social identity model of deindividuation effects. Cogent Psychology, 4(1), 1308104.

Home | About Us | Privacy Policy | Advertise | Contact Us

Simply Psychology's content is for informational and educational purposes only. Our website is not intended to be a substitute for professional medical advice, diagnosis, or treatment.

© Simply Scholar Ltd - All rights reserved

Loss of self-awareness and self-restraint in group situations that foster arousal and anonymity

What is the term for the loss of self

Deindividuation is a concept in social psychology that is generally thought of as the loss of self-awareness in groups, although this is a matter of contention (see below). Sociologists also study the phenomenon of deindividuation, but the level of analysis is somewhat different.

What is the meaning of deindividuation?

deindividuation, phenomenon in which people engage in seemingly impulsive, deviant, and sometimes violent acts in situations in which they believe they cannot be personally identified (e.g., in groups and crowds and on the Internet).

How do you reduce deindividuation?

As Diener's Halloween study showed, people are less likely to behave in irresponsible ways if their identity is known—so one way to reduce deindividuation is to do what the experimenter in this study did: have people be identifiable rather than anonymous. Another approach involves increasing self-awareness.

Who is known for conducting experiments that illustrated social loafing?

STANLEY MILGRAM'S EXPERIMENT. Conformity is one effect of the influence of others on our thoughts, feelings, and behaviors.