What is the best way to describe the relationship between the personality traits?

  • PDFView PDF

Under a Creative Commons license

Open access

Abstract

The purpose of this study is to determine the relationship between personality traits and job engagement of the university personnel. A self-administered survey was applied to Kastamonu University academic and administrative personnel. A relevant data was collected from 118 personnel. The results of the study indicated that there were significant relationships between personality traits and job engagement. Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Openness to Experience were positively related but Neuroticism was negatively related to physical, emotional and cognitive engagement. Openness to Experience was the significant predictor of physical, emotional and cognitive engagement. Agreeableness was the predictor of emotional and cognitive engagement. As a higher order factor job engagement was related to big five factors. Openness to Experience and Agreeableness were the only significant predictors of job engagement. Results of this study suggest that the five factor model is useful for examining the dispositional source of job engagement.

Keywords

Personality

Engagement;

Cited by [0]

Copyright © 2014 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.

Introduction

Social sciences attach great importance to values as constructs that help to understand human attitudes, views, norms and behaviour. In the 1950s, Allport1 pointed out that an individual’s preferable values direct their behaviour, while priorities within values influence the perception of reality. At a later time, the key role of values in giving meaning to single acts and creating a vision of the world was emphasized by Rokeach2,3 and Schwartz.4,5 Such views are widely documented in the literature.6–8

If values are assigned such a large role in the perception of the world and the determination of human behaviour, the following question becomes relevant: what factors determine their hierarchy? What an individual considers valuable is a consequence of their choices. Furthermore the choice of values is conditioned by geographical, climatic, political, economic, historical and legal variables. It depends on cultural, institutional and personality factors.3,9–15 The relationship between values and personality traits will be the subject of empirical consideration. The article uses some of the data from previous publications.16–18

An Analysis of Relations Between Personality Traits and Values

Personality is understood as characteristic patterns of thoughts, feelings and actions which result in specific ways of interacting with the environment.19 One of the concepts of personality is the trait theory. Traits are “psychological entities that can only be inferred from behaviour and experience”.20 They are considered to be the basic components of personality that foster the characteristics of an individual. They serve to clarify observed regularities and the consistency of behaviour and differences among people. Whereas values are most often interpreted as abstract concepts or beliefs [cognitive structure] which refer to preferable end-states of existence or modes of behaviour. They are organized according to the scale of their relative importance and they are transcendent in relation to the situation. They guide the evaluation and selection of specific behaviour. If values are in conflict, a person tends to behave according to the one that occupies a higher position in the hierarchy.2,5,9 What are the similarities and differences between personality traits and values? Are they and how are they related to each other?

According to Dollinger et al21 personality traits and values reveal themselves in the functioning of a person in many situations throughout their life. This fact lies at the core of expectations about their mutual dependence. Both constructs are similar in the sense that they direct the emergence of stable differences in human behaviour22 although personality affects behaviour not as strongly as values23.

There are much more differences between the two constructs. Personality traits are stable dispositions; they describe “what people are like” and they are not always associated with the excitation of motivational tension. Values are also stable but they determine what goals people want to achieve and what behaviour they consider appropriate. The motivational aspect is therefore clearly emphasized. Personality traits guide behaviour but do not constitute their standard. Rather, they are associated with a natural tendency to a certain type of reaction. Values, on the other hand, provide standards in the choice of behaviour. People can explain their behaviour by referring both to traits and values, but only values contain the evaluation component. Therefore, they – not traits – serve to evaluate and justify one’s behaviour and that of others.5,11,24

Values include the conflict element, ie some are realized at the expense of others, while personality traits are not organized on such a basis, for example, one can express extraversion and agreeableness at the same time.25 This is because values differ in importance, ie relative preference with regard to one another within the system, whereas personality traits vary in intensity and frequency of occurrence. Without creating a system, traits may be independent of each other. The manner in which the two constructs are measured is also different: values emphasize the importance of each of them for the Self, personality traits take into account beliefs about one’s self. Furthermore, traits can be positive and negative, values – as desired states – only positive.24,26

Personality traits are considered to be rather inborn constructs.27 Values, on the other hand, are of learned nature, ie they constitute beliefs that reflect an individual’s adaptation to the needs accepted by the society.2 As Parks and Guay25 indicate, individuals behave in an extravert way [personality] since being an extravert is part of their nature. An individual behaves in honest way [value] because he or she has learned that honesty is important. However, the proportions of what is genetic, cultural, and environmental in both constructs are not entirely determined. Furthermore, the value system is relatively more dynamic than personality traits. It can change when an individual adapts to their new environment.2 Personality traits are characterized by greater durability during life,25,28 as evidenced by high – equalling 0.98 – stability coefficients.25

However, in practice it can be difficult to make distinctions between the analysed constructs. Roccas et al26 indicate that the same terms can refer to both personality traits and values. For example, “competence” may refer to “being competent” [trait] as well as a conviction that the pursuit of competence and its presentation are important [value]. However, it is not always the case that someone who has competency in a certain area is convinced that it is a value. It is also sometimes the case that competence is valued without having it in any area of one’s functioning.

Research on Personality Traits and Values – an Overview

In theoretical considerations, the prevailing view is that personality traits and values are two separate constructs which influence behaviour in an interactive manner.1 This does not mean, however, that they are not correlated with each other. The results of research conducted by Oleś29 point to the content-related compatibility of personality traits [diagnosed by means of Cattell’s 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire] with value preferences. The author observed that the values that are particularly highly accepted are the ones that an individual can realize relatively easily, because they are in line with personality traits. This regularity was also confirmed in other studies. They showed, for example, that extraversion [measured with Eysenck Personality Test] is associated with a higher preference for values connected with contacts with other people,30 intelligence – with higher preference for cognitive values30,31 [the research incorporated the Rokeach Value Survey]. Also Bilsky and Schwartz24 described important relations between personality traits [extraversion and emotionality, diagnosed with the Freiburg Personality Inventory] and value preference [Rokeach Value Survey].

A relatively large number of studies on the identified problem refer to the Big Five theory21,26,28,32–38,39 and use Costa and McCrae’s NEO-PI or NEO-FFI Five Factor Inventory. They diagnose five basic personality traits: neuroticism [emotional stability-instability], extraversion [quality and intensity of human interactions], openness to experience [looking for new experiences for oneself], agreeableness [interpersonal relations in the compassion-hostility dimension] and conscientiousness [an individual’s organization, persistence and motivation in pursuit of the goal]. The meta-analysis of the results – conducted by Parks et al – at first based on eleven25 and subsequently sixty40 studies – confirmed the theoretical assumptions concerning the relation between the above mentioned personality traits and values. The values were diagnosed with the use of the Schwartz Value Survey and incorporated references to the model created by this author, the so-called values circumplex model. According to this model, the values and the types determined on their basis can be located in two bipolar dimensions:5

  • conservatism – openness to change;
  • self-enhancement – self-transcendence.

The first of these dimensions reflects a conflict between the pursuit of stability and the cultivation of traditions [submissive self-limitation] and the pursuit of change and a high value of autonomy manifested in the independence of thinking and acting. The second dimension reflects a conflict between focusing on oneself and concentrating on others. Concentration on oneself is associated with self-promotion, pursuit of dominance and focusing one’s activity on personal success. Focusing on others requires abandoning the egocentric perspective, taking into account the well-being of the other person and promoting their well-being.

The two dimensions described above include certain types of values. And so, “universalism” and “benevolence” are components of the “self-transcendence” dimension, whereas “power” and “achievement” comprise the “self-enhancement” dimension. The “conservatism” dimension encompasses: “tradition”, “conformity” and “security”, whereas the “openness to change” includes “stimulation” and “self-direction”. “Hedonism” belongs to two dimensions: “openness to change” and “self-enhancement”.9

In the light of the meta-analyses made by Parks et al,25,40 the values most frequently and most strongly correlated with two traits: openness to experience and agreeableness. The trait openness to experience was positively related to the following types of values: “self-direction”, “stimulation”, “universalism”, and negatively with such types of values: “tradition”, “conformity” and “security”. Positive correlation coefficients were observed for agreeableness with the following types of values: “benevolence”, “universalism”, “conformity”, “tradition”, and a negative correlation coefficient with the “power” type of values. According to the authors, these relations reflect the sense of the analysed constructs. In the context of the trait openness to experience, people are described as independent and inner-directed, curious about the world, creative, open to new ideas and striving for wisdom and understanding of the world. It is compatible with the types of values “self-direction”, “stimulation” and “universalism”. Agreeable people are friendly, loyal, cooperating, supporting each other. The importance of the values “benevolence” and “universalism”, on the other hand, is associated with the conviction that people strive to become honest, friendly and helpful. The values of “conformity” and “tradition” contribute to reduction of impulsiveness and facilitate community life. In case of conscientiousness and extraversion, weaker correlations with values were observed. Nevertheless, conscientiousness was positively related to the types of values: “conformity”, “achievement” and “security”, and extraversion – with types of values: “stimulation”, “achievement”, “power”, and “hedonism”. Neuroticism turned out to be a trait poorly related to values.

Parks-Leduc et al40 observed that “the strength of relations between traits and values should also be somewhat determined by content similarity when comparing each trait with each value” [p. 6]. Moreover, they claimed that more cognitively based traits are more strongly related to values and more emotionally based traits are less strongly related to values. A similar opinion was presented by Zilling et al. [in: Grankvist, Kajonius]41: the personality trait openness to experience [more cognitive] shows stronger links to values than neuroticism [more emotional].

The results described above are generally consistent with empirical data from the work by Roccas et al26 which was used to determine the compatibility of personality traits and value types. Nevertheless, in some cases the authors observed no compatibility. This was revealed, for example, in relation to the trait openness to experience and the value “power”. The research by Roccas et al26 was conducted in Israel, but it was replicated in other countries, such as Sweden. Grankvist and Caionius41 claim that “traits and values are different constructs and that their relationship is consistent across the two geographic locations and student cohorts” [p. 8].

A lack of content-based consistency between personality traits and values was previously found in the research carried out by Furnham42 in which the Eysenck Personality Test was incorporated. The author pointed out that a person can also hold those values which he or she is not able – due to their individual traits – to realize. For example, an emotionally unstable person considers “internal harmony” an important value, a person with low self-esteem – “social recognition”. According to Adler’s views,34 sometimes people may hold values opposite to their personality traits, compensating for the latter. Saroglou and Muñoz‐García43 observed that personality‐values associations are usually modest. One reason for this discrepancy – to their view – may be the fact that people do not always hold values that correspond to who they are.

A comprehensive description of personality requires “something” more than three [Eysenck’s personality theory] or five factors [Costa and McCrae’s personality theory]. Other personality traits and their relationship to values were also an interesting field of study. Balliet et al22 analysed empathy. The hypothesis that this trait correlates most positively with the “benevolence” value type was confirmed, which has a clear theoretical justification. However, the hypothesis that empathy correlates most negatively with the “achievement” value type was not confirmed. Such correlations were observed with the “power” value. In general, the correlation pattern between empathy and the ten types of values from the Schwartz model was in line with the predictions. The relationship between emotional empathy and values was also investigated by Myyry et al.44 Their work resulted in the confirmation of the hypotheses that this trait positively correlates with the type of values “benevolence” and “universalism” as those the realization of which expresses concern for the well-being of people in the immediate vicinity and people “in general”. Later, the author45 pointed out the relationship between values and various aspects of empathy, treating them as predictors of the level of developing moral judgments [Kohlberg’s cognitive developmental approach]. Relationships between empathy and values were also studied by Persson and Kajonius.46 They observed that empathy was strongly and positively related to altruistic values and negatively to self-enhancing values.

Silfver et al47 drew attention to the role of emotions such as empathy, guilt and shame. They assumed that moral emotions and moral values are linked, while a description of these relationships will help to understand motivation. It appeared that guilt was positively correlated with such types of values as “universalism”, “benevolence”, “tradition”, “conformity” and negatively with the types of values: “power”, “hedonism”, “stimulation” and “autonomy”. The relationship with the two components of empathy was similar: empathic concern and perspective taking [the description of the components of empathy will be presented later in this paper]. Shame and personal distress [personal distress is the third component of empathy] were weakly linked with values. In general, the values belonging to the dimensions of “self-transcendence” and “conservatism” are associated with pro-social tendencies, while the values belonging to the dimensions of “self-enhancement” and “openness to experience” are not [cf. Schwartz’s values circumplex model described above].

Personality Traits and Values from the Point of View of the Cognitive Self-Concept

The analysis of the above dependencies leads to the question about the mechanism thanks to which specific relations between personality traits and values appear. The explanation of this mechanism allows for a more integrative understanding of a person.

According to the assumptions of the cognitive self-concept, man makes a mental self-representation. This leads to the creation of rich and varied knowledge components. Examples of such components are beliefs about specific attributes, ie physical characteristics, personality traits, thinking, feelings, as well as roles, standards, values, personal goals, relations with other people.48–51 They constitute contents of the self-concept. Relationships between these constructs are formed in the course of one’s life: as one acquires the ability to analyse oneself, the construction of the self-concept becomes more and more clearly and confidently defined, internally consistent, and stable over time. The so-called “self-concept clarity” belongs to a class of constructs that focus on the structural aspects of the self-concept [how the knowledge components or specific self-beliefs are organized]. Crocetti and Van Dijak52 made a review of the literature and the results of research conducted by many authors. They claimed that high levels of self-concept clarity have been found to be positively related to the perception of meaning in life and affect balance. It is also an indicator of healthy self-development and psychosocial adjustment.

It should be noted that the Self as a psychological construct includes, among other things, a self-representation of one’s personality traits and a hierarchy of values. In the same people, information contained in cognitive schemes “personality traits” and “values” should be linked particularly strongly, as it is an important aspect of the self-concept. The mechanism underlying this relationship is based on the motivation to maintain self-esteem [striving for a positive Self]. It turns out that people with a high sense of values articulate beliefs about the Self well, and thus are characterized by high levels of self-concept clarity. People with low self-esteem, on the other hand, are characterized by a relatively higher level of indeterminacy, instability and inconsistency.50,53–55 Differences in self-evaluation are related, among other things, to the degree of specifying relations between different elements of knowledge contained in the self-concept.56–62 Undoubtedly, self-esteem should not be taken as the only mental representation of a person’s self-concept.63

If there is a merit-based correspondence between personality traits and values [ie two important components of knowledge about the Self], the chances of realizing these values increase. For example, “an exciting life” is more likely to be realized by an extrovert than an introvert person. Behaviour consistent with one’s held values, in turn, contributes to strengthening self-esteem.2 The organization of knowledge about the Self, in which the acceptance of values being in opposition to personality traits is revealed, may result in low self-esteem. The lack of compatibility between these constructs significantly limits the possibility of achieving what it is considered valuable from an axiological point of view. Such dependencies appear most often in the case of values the realization of which depends on satisfying important needs, eg the need for security. The deprivation of these needs is revealed in the form of the so-called “no-value”. Oleś29 showed that people with emotional disintegration, a stronger inclination to blame and a greater internal tension highly appreciated the value of “inner harmony”.

Research Problem and Hypotheses

This paper attempts to characterize value systems in the context of personality variables, ie empathy, agreeableness, directiveness and Machiavellianism. The authors were particularly interested in the extent to which these constructs are integrated. Bearing in mind that both personality traits and values are organized in the self-concept, it should be expected to find many logical connections between them. To address this problem, personality traits need to be characterized in order to capture similarities in the content of particular traits and values.

Empathy

Empathy is interpreted as a multidimensional construct in its nature, containing both emotional and cognitive components.64–68 Emotional components are expressed in an empathic concern which leads to concentration on the other person’s misfortune and attempts to restore their well-being, and in personal distress which, in turn, is oriented towards the Self and bringing relief in one’s own suffering. The inclination to experience empathic concern and personal distress is conceptualized as personality traits. Nevertheless, the former leads to a constant altruistic motivation that increases social competence, while the latter leads to a constant egoistic motivation that decreases these competences. The cognitive component of empathy, defined as perspective taking, refers to the ability to see the world through the eyes of another person, to imagine what he or she feels in a particular situation. The dispositionally conditioned tendency for perspective taking makes it possible to generate empathic emotions which in turn direct the need for giving aid.67,69–75

Empathy emerges in the early stages of ontogenetic development [already an infant may exhibit instinctive help in the misery of others] and is conditioned by genetic factors and childhood experiences.76,77 Only the affective components of empathy are inherited: the tendency to experience empathic concern and personal distress.78 As Eisenberg et al.79,80 evidenced, individual differences in empathic tendencies are determined by emotional sensitivity and reactivity, which in turn are associated with hereditary [to a large extent] temperamental properties.78 Temperament determines dispositional emotional empathy, but not the aptitude for perspective taking. In the development of empathy, the history of the child-parent relationship is also important. Children learn empathy and pro-social reactions from observing their parents’ emotions.81 Early experiences, including the attachment of infants to their parents, allow to transfer feelings and expectations to later social relationships.81,82

The research on prosocial behaviours has led to the formulation of the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Empathic people are more likely to offer help [and this help is selfless, motivated by concern for the well-being of the other person, “more delicate and less capricious”] and less inclined to manifest aggression or other antisocial behaviour as well as show an increased interest in cooperation in conflict situations.83 Empathic people are more tactful, they communicate better with others, show tolerance to different social groups, see others more positively and express sympathy for the social environment.78 Empathy is considered in the context of moral principles – ie concern and justice – and analysed in the process of moral development.84

Agreeableness

Agreeableness is one of the traits included in the Five-Factor Model of Personality. According to the assumptions of Costa and McCrae,85–87 these traits determine the existence of individual differences between people. They are largely biological [to a large extent inherited], relatively unchanging [stable throughout life] and universal [similar features are found in different cultures and languages]. They exert an overwhelming influence on human life by directing perceptions, thinking, feeling and behaviour.

Agreeableness concerns the trait of an individual’s interpersonal references on a continuum from compassion to antagonism in thoughts, feelings and acts. Those who achieve high scores in agreeableness tend to be good, obliging, friendly, delicate, gentle, modest, straightforward, willing to give in and cooperate. They are characterized by trust and sensitivity to other people, concern for their interests and altruistic attitude.

The results of empirical research indicate that people with high agreeableness levels, compared to people with low agreeableness levels, are more positive about people and have a more positive opinion of their partners in interaction when it comes to experimental situations, show a lower level of prejudice [eg to obese people] and negatively evaluate such prejudice. They are characterized by empathy, see themselves as more similar to others and are able to solve conflicts constructively. They offer help more often [regardless of the intensity of the evoked empathy during the study], and this fact is less dependent on their kinship. In general, agreeableness is related to the desire to belong to others, it affects the motivation and behavioural strategies in different social situations.88,89

Agreeableness plays a role in regulating negative emotions, especially in situations of danger. Its higher rates are associated with a lower probability of depressive symptoms and lower levels of anger and aggression [and above all direct aggression]. This helps to maintain positive interpersonal relationships and eliminate interpersonal conflicts.90,91 Agreeableness has been found to be substantially related to empathy,92,93 allows not only to predict pro-social attitudes,23,88 but is also associated with work ethic94 and – what is more interesting – with achievements in learning.95

Directiveness

The concept of directiveness originates from the interest in the issue of authoritarianism. Ray96,97 claims that one should distinguish the notions of “authoritarian attitudes” [an attitude characterized by respect for authorities] and “authoritarian personality” [a personality trait related to the tendency to dominate over others]. The latter term is associated with “directiveness” and, according to the author, these terms can be used interchangeably.

Directiveness – according to Ray97– is a personality trait that comes down to imposing one’s will on people and thus gaining an advantage over them. At its base lies the belief in the rightness of one’s own conduct [self-confidence]. The constitutive feature of directiveness is aggressive dominance. By dominating the social environment, such people are inclined to destroy positive interpersonal relations.

Dominance orientation means the degree to which a person wishes to surpass others [individual dimension] or the degree to which a person wishes his group to surpass others [social dimension]. There is an acceptance of such a hierarchy among people whose values are in line with the principle “better people deserve more”. People with a tendency to dominate show hostility, are more prone to stereotypes and prejudices and have a weaker tolerance for ambiguity, ie they react poorly to unclear situations. At the same time, they are in favour of aggressive resolution of international conflicts and large military expenditures.98–100 A higher level of dominance orientation was found in feminists than in non-feminists,101 in men than women [in all social strata and all cultures], in educated people with higher social status,97,102 and who are highly motivated in their achievements.103 The propensity for dominance correlates negatively with the scale of social approval and the scale of socialization. On the other hand, the research on directiveness and value system [in Scheler’s framework] indicates that high intensity of directiveness is coupled with higher preference for values of pleasure and lower preference for values of the spirit and values of the holy.97

The functioning of people with a high degree of directiveness has certain consequences in the interpersonal dimension. Dominance and aggression – as one might think – limits or even inhibits human pro-community aspirations based on equality, where understanding and acceptance of other people and the pursuit of acts for their benefit [pro-social behaviour] become important elements. It is less likely to build close emotional ties, connected with friendship, love and affection.

Machiavellianism

Machiavellianism is one of the three traits – along with narcissism and psychopathy – that constitute the dark triad of personality.104–106 This term has become synonymous with cunningness, deceitfulness, falsehood and double-dealing. Machiavellists believe that others should be manipulated and believe in the effects of such treatments.107−109 They are primarily aimed at success,110 a success that allows to gain prestige and present narcissistic grandeur, manifesting both superiority and arrogance, as well as the need for the title. Achievements are considered mainly in individual and material terms.111 Machiavellianism is associated with the pursuit of the manifestation of power112 and the need for dominance.113

The specificity of this type of personality is reflected both in the affective and cognitive sphere. Interpersonal relations do not evoke adequate emotions, in terms of content and intensity. Positive emotions are rare, while indifference or even emotional coolness is dominant.113 Machiavellists are not community-oriented and maintain a distanced attitude towards others. They have been found to exploit groups and defect from them afterward to avoid retaliation.114 They are characterized by the ability to efficiently decode other people’s emotional states, but this has no empathic or decentrational consequences: The Machiavellian “rudeness” manifests itself in a reduced capacity for compassion and the inability to accept someone else’s perspective. People are perceived by them in a suspicious and cynical way as weak, untrustworthy and prone to betrayal.108,115,116

Such Machiavellian traits as resistance to external influences and emotional coolness, favour rationality and unprecedented cognitive control over the environment. Machiavellists are well established in reality and rarely show signs of disorganization. Their cognitive effectiveness, manifested by concentration of attention, accuracy of perception, ability to process and use information, also deserves attention. Efficient combinations of information processes determine intelligent and adequate behaviour.111,112,117

The properties presented above stimulate the need for success and condition its realization. Undoubtedly, success is more obtainable when the perspective of another person is not taken into account, but it is considered from the point of view of its usefulness in achieving one’s goals. What can potentially protect other people from abuse and manipulation are moral values, because their function is to take into account the extra-personal good. Machiavellists implement moral values selectively as long as they do not interfere with their own interests. They often violate the prevailing norms and show indifference in their thinking and acting to conventional morality. The pursuit of success plays a major role, and the question of how to achieve the goals seems to be of little importance.108,118,119

The above passages present the characteristics of four personality traits. As can be seen, they play a role in determining the relationship between Self and Others. Two of them – empathy and agreeableness – lie at the basis of constructing mature interpersonal relationships and generating positive behavioural intentions. They encourage pro-social behaviour aimed at the well-being of other people, as well as the suppression of anti-social behaviour addressed against the environment. In the case of the other two traits – directiveness and Machiavellianism – it is typical to focus on one’s own person, pursue one’s personal interests, without regard for the welfare of other people, and even against their good. Values are also classified according to the criterion “promotion of other people’s well-being – promotion of one’s own well-being”. It is then possible to describe their differentiation in the dimension of allocentrism – idiocentrism. This dimension is identical to the collectivism – individualism dimension, where in the first case the differentiation is analysed at the level of personality, in the second case at the level of cultures.4,9,120 Allocentric values direct the focus on interpersonal relationships and are morally relevant. They are connected with concern for others, pursuit of cooperation, equality and honesty. Idiocentric values, on the other hand, contribute to personal achievement and manifest themselves in a tendency to compete. They are connected on the one hand with independence and hedonistic striving, and on the other hand, with interest in the world and the need for cognitive development.

This study expects to uncover characteristics in the axiological dimension with persons differing in the intensity of empathy, agreeableness, directiveness and Machiavellianism, such that are consistent with the definition of personality. The search for relations between personality and value preferences is connected with the assumption that the functioning of a person constitutes psychological whole and the fact that various elements concerning oneself enter into relations during the creation of the self-concept.121,122 The verified hypotheses assume that:

Hypothesis 1: Empathy and agreeableness correlate positively with allocentric values and negatively with idiocentric values.

Hypothesis 2: Directiveness and Machiavellianism correlate positively with idiocentric values and negatively with allocentric values.

Method

Study design: The survey was questionnaire-based [5 questionnaires]. This made it possible to collect data on four personality traits and a value system.

Research group: The study was conducted among 325 people. The age of the respondents was between 20–24 years [average age indicator

=21.02, standard deviation s=0.48]. The survey was carried out in Poland and was anonymous.

Material: The value preferences were determined with the use of the Rokeach Value Survey in a version requiring the formulation of a ranking.123 The author selected 18 terminal values and 18 instrumental values, placing them on two separate scales. Terminal values refer to desirable end-states of existence [how we like the world to be and where we would like to end up]. Instrumental values refer to preferable modes of behavior [how we want to live in our life]. The respondents’ task was to order their values by assigning appropriate ranks, where rank 1 indicated the strongest acceptance of values and rank 18 – the weakest. On the basis of literature analysis it was proposed to divide 36 values in the Rokeach Value Survey into allocentric and idiocentric ones [justification for the division in: Czerniawska124].

Allocentric values were associated with:

  • protecting the well-being of all people and those with whom an individual enters in direct interaction [the welfare of the group to which an individual belongs]: “a world at peace” [t17], “equality” [t2], “helpful” [i8], “honest” [i9], “forgiving” [i7], “loving” [i14], “responsible” [i17];
  • security of identity groups and respect for tradition/religion: “family security” [t4], “national security” [t9], “salvation” [t11];
  • balanced social views, intrapersonal and interpersonal harmony: “wisdom” [t16], “inner harmony” [t7], “self-controlled” [i18], “clean” [i5], “polite” [i16], “obedient” [i15], “mature love” [t8], “true friendship” [t15].

Idiocentric values were associated with:

  • social status, prestige and personal [including material] success: “social recognition” [t14], “self-respect” [t12], “a sense of accomplishment” [t13], “ambitious” [i1], “a comfortable life” [t1];
  • freedom of choice, independence of thought and action, intellectual competence: “freedom” [t5], “independent” [i11], “courageous” [i6], “imaginative” [i10], “broad-minded” [i2], “capable” [i3], “intellectual” [i12], “logical” [i13];
  • hedonism and the need for stimulation [interesting, pleasant, varied life]: “happiness” [t6], “cheerful” [i4], “pleasure” [t10], “an exciting life” [t3], “a world of beauty” [t18].

The personality traits were diagnosed using:

  • empathy – the Questionnaire of Empathetic Understanding of Other People by Węgliński.125 The indicators range from 0 to 99, with 0 being the minimum intensity of the trait and 99 being the maximum intensity of the trait;
  • agreeableness – the Personality Inventory NEO-FFI by Costa and McCrae.126 The indicators range from 0 to 48, with 0 being the minimum intensity of the trait and 48 being the maximum intensity of the trait;
  • directiveness – the Directiveness Scale D-26 by J.J. Ray.97 The minimum number of points that surveyed person may obtain is 26, the maximum – 78;
  • Machiavellianism – the Mach V Scale by R. Christi and F.L. Geis.110 The indicators range from 40 to 160 points.

Each of the tools used in the research was developed from a psychometric point of view. Detailed information can be found in publications by the following authors: Brzozowski [1989; Rokeach Value Survey], Węgliński [1987; the Questionnaire of Empathetic Understanding of Other People by Węgliński], Drwal, Brzozowski [1995; the Mach V scale by R. Christi and F.L. Geis], Brzozowski [1997; the Directiveness Scale D-26 by J.J. Ray] and Zawadzki et al [1998; the Personality Inventory NEO-FFI by Costa and McCrae].

Procedure: The survey was conducted during a 2.5-hour meeting [groups of 20–30 persons]. The respondents answered the questions in the questionnaires. Handing out of the next questionnaire was preceded by a 15-minute break. The survey was anonymous and participation – voluntary.

Statistical analysis: The following information was obtained for each of the respondents:

  • preference indicators for 18 terminal values,
  • preference indicators for 18 instrumental values,
  • empathy indicator,
  • agreeableness indicator,
  • directiveness indicator,
  • Machiavellism indicator.

Pearson’s linear correlation was used in the statistical analysis. This test allows to establish relationships between the indicated constructs, the existence of which was assumed in the hypotheses.

Results

The statistical analysis was initiated by establishing the relationship of among personality traits. It turned out that the indicators of empathy [the affective aspect of empathy was measured] and agreeableness [r=0.31, p

Chủ Đề