It is possible to increase intrinsic motivation to control prejudice by:

Those who are high in external motivation to respond without prejudice (EMS) tend to focus on non-racial attributes when describing others. This fMRI study examined the neural processing of race and an alternative yet stereotypically relevant attribute (viz., socioeconomic status: SES) as a function of the perceiver’s EMS. Sixty-one White participants privately formed impressions of Black and White faces ascribed with high or low SES. Analyses focused on regions supporting race- and status-based reward/salience (NAcc), evaluation (VMPFC) and threat/relevance (amygdala). Consistent with previous findings from the literature on status-based evaluation, we observed greater neural responses to high-status (vs low-status) targets in all regions of interest when participants were relatively low in EMS. In contrast, we observed the opposite pattern when participants were relatively high in EMS. Notably, all effects were independent of target race. In summary, White perceivers’ race-related motivations similarly altered their neural responses to the SES of Black and White targets. Specifically, the findings suggest that EMS may attenuate the positive value and/or salience of high status in a mixed-race context. Findings are discussed in the context of the stereotypic relationship between race and SES.

Introduction

Race remains a topic that generates considerable tension in the USA. For racial majority members (i.e. Whites), cross-race perception and interaction can elicit threat or anxiety due to concerns about violating egalitarian norms (Plant and Devine, 1998, 2003; Richeson and Shelton, 2003; Richeson and Trawalter, 2008; Amodio, 2009), even when such norms are explicitly endorsed (Ickes, 1984; Gaertner and Dovidio, 1986; Devine et al., 1996). In contrast to individuals who intentionally cultivate an egalitarian self-concept (i.e. internal motivation to respond without prejudice, IMS: Amodio et al., 2003, 2008), individuals who are motivated to avoid the social sanctions of expressing prejudice (i.e. external motivation to respond without prejudice, EMS: Butz and Plant, 2009; Olson and Zabel, 2015) can be especially uncomfortable when race is salient (Amodio et al., 2006). Potentially due to race-related discomfort, high-EMS Whites typically engage in more effortful (albeit less efficient) self-regulation during intergroup interactions (Lambert et al., 2003; Richeson et al., 2003; Richeson and Shelton, 2003; Hausmann and Ryan, 2004; Wyer, 2007; Ito et al., 2015). High-EMS individuals also tend to avoid explicit mentions of race, focusing instead on non-racial categories or topics (Norton et al., 2006; Apfelbaum et al., 2008). This study examines the neural responses to perceived race and non-racial attributes (viz., socioeconomic status: SES) as a function of White perceivers’ EMS levels.

In this study, we aimed to address two key gaps in the literature on race and status. Although previous work has found that high-EMS Whites favor the use of perceived non-racial attributes (e.g. age and gender) over race when describing people (Apfelbaum et al., 2008), it is unclear whether and how high-EMS individuals use relatively more knowledge-based attributes (e.g. SES), to inform interracial impressions. SES is particularly relevant in the context of race due to evidence of race–SES associations (Allport, 1954; Weeks and Lupfer, 2004; Penner and Saperstein, 2008; Freeman et al., 2011; Sanchez and Garcia, 2012; Brannon and Markus, 2013). Despite this evidence, few studies have explored how race and status jointly affect person evaluation (Smedley and Bayton, 1978; Fiske et al., 2016; Richeson and Sommers, 2016). Additionally, few fMRI studies have considered effects of motivation on evaluative bias (but see Wheeler and Fiske, 2005; Li et al., 2016), and none have examined the role of EMS to our knowledge. This is surprising given the extensive behavioral and electrophysiological literature on this important motivation (Blascovich et al., 2001; Mendes et al., 2002; Plant and Devine, 2003; Paolini et al., 2004; Amodio et al., 2006; Mendes et al., 2007; Page-Gould et al., 2008, 2010; Amodio, 2009; Bijleveld et al., 2012; Trawalter et al., 2012). Addressing these gaps in the literature, we examine how EMS shapes neural responses during evaluations of targets varying in race and SES in key regions believed to support reward/salience (ventral striatum: VS), person evaluation (VMPFC) and threat/relevance (amygdala).

The VS and its cortical afferents (e.g. OFC, VMPFC and amygdala) are responsive to various aspects of reward (Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Ruff and Fehr, 2014), including the evaluation of rewarding perceptual (Cloutier et al., 2008) and knowledge-based (Delgado et al., 2005) cues. The VS is also sensitive to personal and emotional salience more broadly (Phan et al., 2004; Cooper and Knutson, 2008), showing attenuated responses during the regulation of negative affect (Phan et al., 2005). Consistent with the VS’s sensitivity to reward and salience, VS activity is modulated by both social status and race (Zink et al., 2008; Ly et al., 2011; Stanley et al., 2012; Ligneul and Dreher, 2017). For example, Stanley et al. (2012) found that participants who trusted Blacks less than Whites showed greater VS activity (viz., NAcc) to trusted Blacks, perhaps reflecting the salience of reward-related outcomes (positive or negative) in uncertain contexts (Cooper and Knutson, 2008; Knutson et al., 2008). Increased VS activity has also been found to high (vs low) status in competence-based hierarchies (Zink et al., 2008), corroborating work from the animal literature on the intrinsic reward value (Deaner et al., 2005) and salience (Shepherd et al., 2006) of high status. Taken together, these findings indicate that the VS responds to the reward value or socioemotional salience of others, with greater responses to more rewarding/salient targets.

The VMPFC is believed to support the elaboration of affective meaning (Roy et al., 2012; Delgado et al., 2016). During person evaluation, the VMPFC is responsive the perceiver’s degree of background knowledge and/or experience with the target (Gilbert et al., 2012; Flagan and Beer, 2013) and has been posited to index positive evaluations based on social status (Cloutier et al., 2012, 2016; Cloutier and Gyurovski, 2014). On this account, the VMPFC should respond similarly to the VS, showing greater responses to targets paired with positive knowledge-based attributes (viz., high status). Given little evidence of race effects in the VMPFC, it is unclear if this region is also involved in race-based evaluations.

The amygdala responds to arousing and biologically relevant stimuli (Phelps and LeDoux, 2005; Adolphs, 2010) and is sensitive to perceived race (Kubota et al., 2012). Like for the VS, amygdala activity is attenuated during the regulation of negative affect (Phan et al., 2005). In the absence of additional information, White perceivers show greater and more sustained amygdala activity for Black than White targets (Hart et al., 2000; Phelps et al., 2000; Ronquillo et al., 2007), especially for perceivers with greater implicit anti-Black associations (Phelps et al., 2000; Cunningham et al., 2004; Brosch et al., 2013). However, when White perceivers are given sufficient time (Cunningham et al., 2004) or instructions to form impressions of Black targets (Wheeler and Fiske, 2005), amygdala sensitivity to race has not been observed (Chekroud et al., 2014). Additionally, race-related amygdala activity is diminished when faces display an averted (vs direct) gaze, presumably posing less relevance or threat to the perceiver (Richeson et al., 2008). Although the amygdala has received considerably less attention in the fMRI literature on status, findings suggest that the amygdala is sensitive to high-status targets, unstable hierarchies and status-based learning and memory (Mattan et al., 2017).

In this study, we examined regions of interest (ROIs) implicated in reward/salience (NAcc in the VS), social evaluation (VMPFC) and threat/relevance (amygdala) as White perceivers formed impressions of faces varying in race and SES. In particular, we assessed whether these regions were responsive to individual differences in EMS.

Based on the literature reviewed earlier, one possibility is that race and SES have independent effects on person evaluation. Because high-EMS White perceivers find perceiving Black targets potentially aversive, Black targets should diminish activity in NAcc (i.e. reward/salience) and VMPFC (i.e. social evaluation) and potentially increase amygdala (i.e. threat/relevance) responses. We also expected that high (vs low) SES would elicit greater activity in the NAcc and VMPFC (Mattan et al., 2017). However, it was unclear how EMS would affect perceived SES. Some research suggests that high-EMS Whites may be motivated to favor non-racial attributes or topics over race (Norton et al., 2006; Apfelbaum et al., 2008). Accordingly, an EMS-related preference for non-racial attributes may enhance the evaluative significance of high SES. Alternatively, other work suggests that EMS biases visual attention toward rather than away from race (Richeson and Trawalter, 2008; Bean et al., 2012). From this perspective, the salience of other social dimensions (e.g. SES) may diminish, thereby attenuating the usual pattern of status-based neural evaluation (i.e. high > low: Mattan et al., 2017).

Another possibility consistent with previously observed categorical associations between race and status (Weeks and Lupfer, 2004; Penner and Saperstein, 2008; Freeman et al., 2011; Lei and Bodenhausen, 2017) is that race and status may interact during person evaluation (Correll et al., 2011; Moore-Berg et al., 2017). Due to high-EMS perceivers’ anxiety about appearing prejudiced (Amodio et al., 2006) and tendency to focus on non-racial attributes (Norton et al., 2006; Apfelbaum et al., 2008), NAcc and VMPFC responses may evince a simultaneously enhanced evaluation of high-SES Black targets and a devaluation of high-SES White targets (Bergsieker et al., 2010; Swencionis and Fiske, 2016)—with the reverse pattern in amygdala activation. In other words, the race by SES interaction should result in a larger effect of SES for Black (vs White) targets in the entire network of brain regions supporting person evaluation.

Materials and methods

Participants

Chicago-area men were recruited via online ads, fliers and banners on public transportation; 82 participants passed initial screening (Supplementary Material S1). Of the 82 eligible participants, 61 completed the study. One participant was excluded from analyses as an outlier for IMS (a control variable), exceeding 3.5 s.d. from the sample means (see Results). The final sample comprised 60 male participants (Mage = 23.8 years, s.d.age = 4.59 years). Participants provided informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the University of Chicago IRB.

Protocol

Key measures and procedures are reported later. However, a complete account of measures and procedures is provided in Supplementary Materials S1 and S2.

Online pretesting session

Eligible participants completed a battery of questionnaires online. Most of these measures were assessed for a large-scale resting-state fMRI investigation or an unrelated experiment completed immediately prior to the impression-formation task.

fMRI session

Upon pretest completion, participants were scheduled; on the day of scanning, participants were instructed to arrive without having consumed drugs including caffeine and alcohol.

Pre-scan

After signing consent and imaging center paperwork, the participant was photographed and completed surveys. Before entering the scanner, participants were trained on the impression-formation task.

Scanning

Participants first completed two fMRI runs of an unrelated task. After this task, participants completed a brief reminder task of the status–color associations and button box responding. All participants correctly recalled the status–color associations. After this reminder, participants completed two runs of the impression-formation task (each ∼4 min), followed by resting-state and anatomical scans, time permitting (total scan time ∼1 h).

Post-scan

Participants completed explicit stimulus ratings and judgments. After this block of surveys, participants were compensated and debriefed.

Status–color association training

Participants learned that this study was interested in how people think of others varying in SES. SES was defined as follows: ‘those who have the highest social status tend to have the most money, the most education and the most respected jobs. Those who have the lowest social status tend to have the least money, the least education and the least respected jobs or no job’. Following this definition, participants learned to associate colors with low- and high-status Americans (e.g. blue = low and orange = high). Status–color associations were counterbalanced across participants.

To thoroughly learn status–color associations, participants completed simple association training blocks (Mitchell et al., 2004, 2005; Cloutier et al., 2013; Cloutier and Gyurovski, 2014). In an initial block of 10 trials, participants viewed a darkened silhouette over a colored background (i.e. orange or blue: five per status level), indicating by key press whether the silhouette was low or high status based on the background color. Participants were informed of their cumulative accuracy on each trial (M = 98.5%). Next, participants completed a block of 10 trials (five per status level) in which they were asked what color represents low (or high) status. Participants were again informed of their cumulative accuracy on each trial (M = 93.4%).

Measures

Impression-formation task

Having learned the two status–color associations, participants briefly practiced the impression-formation task before scanning. The experimenter first verbally confirmed that the participant learned the status-color associations and then explained that participants would no longer be categorizing targets as low or high in status for the impression-formation task. Instead, they would be forming quick overall impressions of male faces, taking into account all visually available information (Cloutier and Gyurovski, 2014). This was repeated for participants in the written instructions for the practice block of the impression-formation task.

The procedures for the practice and functional scanning blocks were identical and adhered to a rapid event-related design (Friston et al., 1999). Trials began with a male face surrounded by a colored frame over a black background. After 1500 ms, the face was replaced by a white fixation of a jittered duration (i.e. intertrial interval of 500, 2500, 4500 or 6500 ms). Participants formed a quick impression of each individual by the time the face disappeared or shortly thereafter. To signal they formed an impression, participants simultaneously pressed two keys, one per index finger. Participants were informed that their responses were not meant to indicate of the content of their impressions but merely to indicate that they had formed an impression. In each run of the impression-formation task, participants viewed 60 male faces divided evenly across conditions (see Supplementary Material S3 for details on stimulus equating). Two reminder trials after the first and second thirds of the sequence required participants to identify the status level of a silhouette framed by either blue or orange.

Faces from all four conditions were interspersed in a fixed pseudorandom sequence. To optimize fMRI design efficiency (Dale, 1999), three fixed trial sequences were generated using optseq2 (Greve, 2002: see Supplementary Material S3). After counterbalancing first color order (i.e. blue vs orange) and status–color associations, four versions of the impression-formation task were generated for each trial sequence. Over the course of the experiment, participants completed one corresponding counterbalanced version from each trial sequence (i.e. the practice block and two experimental runs).

Motivation to respond without prejudice

This 10-item measure administered online during the pretest block assesses external (EMS—5 items) and internal (IMS—5 items) motivations to avoid prejudice toward Black Americans (Plant and Devine, 1998) on a 9-point scale from 1 = strongly disagree to 9 = strongly agree. EMS and IMS were uncorrelated in the final sample, r(59) = 0.052, P = 0.694. For details on the distributions of EMS and IMS, see Supplementary Material S4.

fMRI acquisition and GLM

We used a Phillips dStream Achieva 3T system and 32-channel head coil to acquire BOLD, T2* contrast-weighted EPIs. With a 2000 ms repetition time and a 25 ms echo time, we acquired 34 oblique slices using an interleaved z-shim acquisition protocol (Du et al., 2007). Slices were 4 mm thick with a 0.5 mm gap, a 3 mm2 in-plane resolution, 77° flip angle and a 192 × 134 × 192 mm field of view. Slices were aligned to the AC–PC axis of each participant (Deichmann et al., 2003).

EPIs from each participant’s two runs were preprocessed and analyzed using SPM8 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm), facilitated by a custom suite of scripts for fMRI analysis (https://github.com/ddwagner/SPM8w). We first implemented slice-time correction (Sladky et al., 2011), using the 17th slice acquisition as the reference. Subsequently, we integrated the four repeated z-shim slices (Du et al., 2007). The resulting images from each participant were then unwarped and realigned to the participant’s mean EPI to correct for motion and motion-by-distortion interactions (Andersson et al., 2001). Images were subsequently normalized to the MNI template and smoothed with an 8 mm FWHM kernel (Ashburner and Friston, 1999).

To estimate the BOLD responses for each condition, each trial was considered as an event, and the stimulus time series was convolved with the canonical hemodynamic response function (HRF). (During the review process, parallel analyses were conducted modeling the temporal and dispersion derivatives in addition to the canonical HRF. Results—available upon request—were similar to the model reported here.) A GLM modeled both scan sequences concatenated as a single session with regressors for each of the four conditions (ordered as follows: high-status Black, high-status White, low-status Black and low-status White) followed by regressors controlling for variance associated with: (i) reminder trials; (ii) low-frequency drift (i.e. a linear trend); (iii) session means (1 for scan 1, 0 for scan 2); (iv) six movement parameters; (v) a constant across all scans and (vi) slow fluctuation of the signal (i.e. a standard set of harmonic regressors effectively serving as a 1/128-Hz high-pass filter). Contrast images reflecting the first-level effects of interest were used in second-level analyses.

Analysis

Primary analyses focused on correlations between EMS and task-related activity in key ROIs. Additionally, we conducted exploratory whole-brain regression analyses, testing for activity that correlated with EMS. In all analyses, we controlled for IMS by modeling it as an independent covariate of non-interest. Because mean IMS was 7.64 (on a scale from 1 to 9), all models assume a high-IMS participant sample. For all analyses, the pattern of findings was similar with or without the IMS covariate. Full results from the whole-brain analyses as well as group-level contrasts for effects of race, status and the Race × Status interaction are reported in Supplementary Material S4.

ROI analyses

Guided by previous neuroimaging work on status-based person evaluation, we extracted BOLD activity from ROIs in the VMPFC, MNIx, y, z = [0, 52, −6] (Cloutier et al., 2012), left NAcc, MNIx, y, z = [−9, 8, −8] and right NAcc, MNIx, y, z = [9, 14, −6] (Cloutier et al., 2008) and left amygdala, MNIx, y, z = [−24, −3, −12] and right amygdala, MNIx, y, z = [24, −3, −21] (Cloutier et al., 2014). Average parameter estimates (vs baseline) were extracted for each condition from an 8 mm sphere (VMPFC) or a 4 mm sphere (NAcc, amygdalae).

ROIs were analyzed using the lme4 package for linear mixed-effects models (Bates et al., 2015) in R (R Core Team, 2017). Degrees of freedom were estimated using Satterthwaite’s approximation, provided by the package lmerTest, version 2.0–33 (Kuznetsova et al., 2016). Face stimulus coding for all models was as follows: Blacks = −0.5, Whites = 0.5, low status = −0.5 and high status = 0.5. EMS was treated as a continuous predictor. Random intercepts were modeled to account for participant-level variations in average neural response. For each ROI, we examined whether race, status, EMS and all possible interactions predicted neural activity.

Exploratory whole-brain regressions

We conducted three separate second-level GLMs for (i) the main effect of race, (ii) the main effect of status and (iii) the Race × Status interaction (see Supplementary Material S4). Similar to the ROI analyses, second-level GLMs examined correlations between each effect and EMS. For all whole-brain analyses, we used a voxel-level threshold of P < 0.001 with a cluster extent threshold of 53 voxels as determined by AlphaSim. It should be noted that cluster-level thresholds generated by AlphaSim do not adequately control for false positives in all conditions, especially for parametric analyses (Eklund et al., 2016). Therefore, these exploratory analyses should be interpreted with caution, pending future replication.

Results

ROI analyses

NAcc

Because results from the left and right NAcc largely converged, we report them together. As predicted, results revealed a significant main effect of status, with a greater neural response to high-status compared with low-status faces in left, b = 0.352, SE = 0.121, CI95% = [0.115, 0.590], t(174) = 2.910, P = 0.004, and right NAcc, b = 0.457, SE = 0.165, CI95% = [0.134, 0.780], t(174) = 2.775, P = 0.006. Critically, this status effect was significantly modulated by EMS in the left, b = −0.0668, SE = 0.0221, CI95% = [−0.110, −0.0235], t(174) = −3.021, P = 0.003, and right NAcc, b = −0.0604, SE = 0.0301, CI95% = [−0.146, −0.0284], t(174) = −2.903, P = 0.004. All other effects were non-significant, P > 0.11 and > 0.16 for the left and right NAcc, respectively.

To decompose the Status × EMS interaction, a series of follow-up models tested for: (i) the simple slopes of EMS for each status level and (ii) the simple effects of status for participants at varying levels of EMS. Collapsing across race, these models predicted left and right NAcc response as a function of status, EMS and Status × EMS. Separately for low status (low = 1, high = 0) and high status (low = 0, high = 1), we evaluated the earlier model at low (−1.5 s.d.), mean and high (+1.5 s.d.) EMS (Figure 1). Statistics for the left and right NAcc are reported in Tables 1 and 2, respectively.

Table 1.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the left NAcc

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.042 0.020 [0.003, 0.081] 2.129 114 0.035* High status −0.025 0.020 [−0.063, 0.014] −1.260 114 0.209 High–low status Low EMS 0.220 0.082 [0.059, 0.381] 2.675 178 0.008* Mean EMS 0.013 0.045 [−0.074, 0.106] 0.290 178 0.772 High EMS −0.194 0.082 [−0.355, −0.033] −2.360 178 0.020* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.042 0.020 [0.003, 0.081] 2.129 114 0.035* High status −0.025 0.020 [−0.063, 0.014] −1.260 114 0.209 High–low status Low EMS 0.220 0.082 [0.059, 0.381] 2.675 178 0.008* Mean EMS 0.013 0.045 [−0.074, 0.106] 0.290 178 0.772 High EMS −0.194 0.082 [−0.355, −0.033] −2.360 178 0.020* 

Table 1.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the left NAcc

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.042 0.020 [0.003, 0.081] 2.129 114 0.035* High status −0.025 0.020 [−0.063, 0.014] −1.260 114 0.209 High–low status Low EMS 0.220 0.082 [0.059, 0.381] 2.675 178 0.008* Mean EMS 0.013 0.045 [−0.074, 0.106] 0.290 178 0.772 High EMS −0.194 0.082 [−0.355, −0.033] −2.360 178 0.020* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.042 0.020 [0.003, 0.081] 2.129 114 0.035* High status −0.025 0.020 [−0.063, 0.014] −1.260 114 0.209 High–low status Low EMS 0.220 0.082 [0.059, 0.381] 2.675 178 0.008* Mean EMS 0.013 0.045 [−0.074, 0.106] 0.290 178 0.772 High EMS −0.194 0.082 [−0.355, −0.033] −2.360 178 0.020* 

Table 2.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the right NAcc

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.013 0.027 [−0.040, 0.065] 0.466 113 0.642 High status −0.075 0.027 [−0.128, −0.022] −2.780 113 0.006* High–low status Low EMS 0.284 0.112 [0.065, 0.503] 2.538 178 0.012* Mean EMS 0.014 0.062 [−0.107, 0.134] 0.219 178 0.827 High EMS −0.257 0.112 [−0.476, −0.038] −2.300 178 0.023* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.013 0.027 [−0.040, 0.065] 0.466 113 0.642 High status −0.075 0.027 [−0.128, −0.022] −2.780 113 0.006* High–low status Low EMS 0.284 0.112 [0.065, 0.503] 2.538 178 0.012* Mean EMS 0.014 0.062 [−0.107, 0.134] 0.219 178 0.827 High EMS −0.257 0.112 [−0.476, −0.038] −2.300 178 0.023* 

Table 2.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the right NAcc

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.013 0.027 [−0.040, 0.065] 0.466 113 0.642 High status −0.075 0.027 [−0.128, −0.022] −2.780 113 0.006* High–low status Low EMS 0.284 0.112 [0.065, 0.503] 2.538 178 0.012* Mean EMS 0.014 0.062 [−0.107, 0.134] 0.219 178 0.827 High EMS −0.257 0.112 [−0.476, −0.038] −2.300 178 0.023* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS Low status 0.013 0.027 [−0.040, 0.065] 0.466 113 0.642 High status −0.075 0.027 [−0.128, −0.022] −2.780 113 0.006* High–low status Low EMS 0.284 0.112 [0.065, 0.503] 2.538 178 0.012* Mean EMS 0.014 0.062 [−0.107, 0.134] 0.219 178 0.827 High EMS −0.257 0.112 [−0.476, −0.038] −2.300 178 0.023* 

It is possible to increase intrinsic motivation to control prejudice by:

Left (a) and right (b) NAcc response as a function of status level at low (−1.5 s.d.), mean and high (+1.5 s.d.) EMS. Significant slopes (†) and significant simple effects (*) are indicated, P < 0.05.

Summary

Consistent with previous findings (Zink et al., 2008), we observed greater NAcc responses to high-status (vs low-status) targets but only at low-EMS levels. At high levels of EMS, we observed the opposite—NAcc responses were greater to low-status (vs high-status) targets. NAcc responses did not show any effects of race.

VMPFC

Results revealed a significant main effect of status, with greater neural response to high status than low status, b = 0.947, SE = 0.358, CI95% = [0.245, 1.649], t(174) = 2.646, P = 0.009. This status effect was significantly modulated by EMS, b = −0.187, SE = 0.0654, CI95% = [−0.315, −0.0584], t(174) = −2.853, P = 0.005. All other effects were non-significant, P > 0.15.

To decompose the Status × EMS interaction, a series of follow-up models tested for: (i) the simple slopes of EMS for each level of status and (ii) the simple effects of status for participants at varying levels of EMS. Collapsing across race, these models predicted VMPFC response as a function of status, EMS and Status × EMS. Separately for low status (low = 1, high = 0) and high status (low = 0, high = 1), we evaluated the earlier model at low (−1.5 s.d.), mean and high (+1.5 s.d.) EMS (Figure 2). Simple slopes and effects statistics are reported in Table 3.

Table 3.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the VMPFC

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status −0.122 0.078 [−0.275, 0.031] −1.56 82 0.123 Low status 0.064 0.078 [−0.088, 0.218] 0.829 82 0.410 High–low status Low EMS 0.577 0.241 [0.106, 1.049] 2.399 178 0.017* Mean EMS −0.000 0.133 [−0.260, 0.260] 0.000 178 0.999 High EMS −0.578 0.241 [−1.049, −0.106] −2.400 178 0.017* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status −0.122 0.078 [−0.275, 0.031] −1.56 82 0.123 Low status 0.064 0.078 [−0.088, 0.218] 0.829 82 0.410 High–low status Low EMS 0.577 0.241 [0.106, 1.049] 2.399 178 0.017* Mean EMS −0.000 0.133 [−0.260, 0.260] 0.000 178 0.999 High EMS −0.578 0.241 [−1.049, −0.106] −2.400 178 0.017* 

Table 3.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the VMPFC

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status −0.122 0.078 [−0.275, 0.031] −1.56 82 0.123 Low status 0.064 0.078 [−0.088, 0.218] 0.829 82 0.410 High–low status Low EMS 0.577 0.241 [0.106, 1.049] 2.399 178 0.017* Mean EMS −0.000 0.133 [−0.260, 0.260] 0.000 178 0.999 High EMS −0.578 0.241 [−1.049, −0.106] −2.400 178 0.017* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status −0.122 0.078 [−0.275, 0.031] −1.56 82 0.123 Low status 0.064 0.078 [−0.088, 0.218] 0.829 82 0.410 High–low status Low EMS 0.577 0.241 [0.106, 1.049] 2.399 178 0.017* Mean EMS −0.000 0.133 [−0.260, 0.260] 0.000 178 0.999 High EMS −0.578 0.241 [−1.049, −0.106] −2.400 178 0.017* 

It is possible to increase intrinsic motivation to control prejudice by:

VMPFC response as a function of target status at low (−1.5 s.d.), mean and high (+1.5 s.d.) EMS. Significant simple effects (*) are indicated, P < 0.05.

Summary

Overall, results in the VMPFC mirrored findings in the bilateral NAcc. Consistent with our review of status-based evaluation (Mattan et al., 2017), we observed greater VMPFC responses to high-status (vs low-status) targets but only at low-EMS levels. At high levels of EMS, we observed the opposite—VMPFC responses were greater to low-status (vs high-status) targets. VMPFC responses did not show any effects of race.

Amygdalae

In the amygdala, all significant effects were right-lateralized. Specifically, we found a significant main effect of status, with a greater neural response to high status than low status, b = 0.408, SE = 0.197, CI95% = [0.022, 0.794], t(174) = 2.073, P = 0.040. This status effect was significantly modulated by EMS, b = −0.086, SE = 0.0360, CI95% = [−0.156, −0.0152], t(174) = −2.381, P = 0.018. All other effects in the bilateral amygdala were non-significant, P > 0.12.

To decompose the Status × EMS interaction, a series of follow-up models tested for: (i) the simple slopes of EMS for each level of status and (ii) the simple effects of status for participants at varying levels of EMS. Collapsing across race, these models predicted right amygdala response as a function of status, EMS and Status × EMS. Separately for low status (low = 1, high = 0) and high status (low = 0, high = 1), we evaluated the earlier model at low (−1.5 s.d.), mean and high (+1.5 s.d.) EMS (Figure 3). Simple slopes and effects statistics are reported in Table 4.

Table 4.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the right amygdala

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status 0.030 0.050 [−0.068, 0.128] 0.593 75 0.555 Low status 0.115 0.050 [0.017, 0.213] 2.307 75 0.024* High–low status Low EMS 0.238 0.133 [−0.022, 0.499] 1.793 178 0.075 Mean EMS −0.027 0.073 [−0.170, 0.117] −0.360 178 0.716 High EMS −0.292 0.133 [−0.552, −0.031] −2.200 178 0.029* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status 0.030 0.050 [−0.068, 0.128] 0.593 75 0.555 Low status 0.115 0.050 [0.017, 0.213] 2.307 75 0.024* High–low status Low EMS 0.238 0.133 [−0.022, 0.499] 1.793 178 0.075 Mean EMS −0.027 0.073 [−0.170, 0.117] −0.360 178 0.716 High EMS −0.292 0.133 [−0.552, −0.031] −2.200 178 0.029* 

Table 4.

Contrast statistics for Status × EMS interaction in the right amygdala

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status 0.030 0.050 [−0.068, 0.128] 0.593 75 0.555 Low status 0.115 0.050 [0.017, 0.213] 2.307 75 0.024* High–low status Low EMS 0.238 0.133 [−0.022, 0.499] 1.793 178 0.075 Mean EMS −0.027 0.073 [−0.170, 0.117] −0.360 178 0.716 High EMS −0.292 0.133 [−0.552, −0.031] −2.200 178 0.029* 

Analyses . Group . b . SE . CI95% . t . df . P . High–low EMS High status 0.030 0.050 [−0.068, 0.128] 0.593 75 0.555 Low status 0.115 0.050 [0.017, 0.213] 2.307 75 0.024* High–low status Low EMS 0.238 0.133 [−0.022, 0.499] 1.793 178 0.075 Mean EMS −0.027 0.073 [−0.170, 0.117] −0.360 178 0.716 High EMS −0.292 0.133 [−0.552, −0.031] −2.200 178 0.029* 

It is possible to increase intrinsic motivation to control prejudice by:

Right amygdala response as a function of target status at low (−1.5 s.d.), mean and high (+1.5 s.d.) EMS. Significant slopes (†) and significant simple effects (*) are indicated, P < 0.05.

Summary

Overall, activity in the right amygdala was similar to the observed patterns in the NAcc and VMPFC. At low-EMS levels, we observed similar amygdala responses to high- and low-status targets. At high levels of EMS, amygdala responses were greater to low-status (vs high-status) targets. Consistent with research failing to observe race differences in amygdala responses when person knowledge is present (Chekroud et al., 2014), the amygdalae did not show any reliable effects of race.

Exploratory whole-brain regressions

Results revealed a number of significant clusters correlating with the main effect of status but none for any effects implicating race. We observed some convergent evidence for status effects in the VMPFC/OFC (56 voxels: Figure 4). At a more liberal voxel-wise threshold of P < 0.005 (k > 117 voxels), this significant cluster increased to 469 voxels, extending into the bilateral NAcc. Full details of exploratory whole-brain regressions are reported in Supplementary Material S4.

It is possible to increase intrinsic motivation to control prejudice by:

Whole-brain regressions targeting status revealed a 56-voxel cluster in the VMPFC (MNIx, y, z = [6, 39, −6]) that corroborated the Status × EMS interaction found in the VMPFC ROI analysis (voxel-wise threshold, P < 0.001, extent threshold of k = 53 voxels as determined by AlphaSim).

Discussion

The present research found that EMS modulated the processing of a non-racial attribute (viz., status) in a network of brain regions involved in person evaluation. Consistent with previous findings, we observed greater responses to high-status (vs low-status) targets in the bilateral NAcc and VMPFC but only at low-EMS levels. The opposite pattern (low > high) was observed at high levels of EMS in the bilateral NAcc, VMPFC and right amygdala. Notably, these effects of status were independent of target race, consistent with previous findings suggesting that perceived race and status can, in some instances, contribute independently to person evaluations (Smedley and Bayton, 1978; Blascovich et al., 2001; Mattan et al., under review).

Status effects

Results were consistent with previous work on the neural evaluation of social status (Mattan et al., 2017). Findings from the behavioral literature have shown that high-status targets are generally evaluated more positively (Fiske, 2010; Varnum, 2013). Consistent with this picture, high-status (vs low-status) targets elicited greater activity in regions that have previously been implicated in processing reward/salience (NAcc: Berridge and Kringelbach, 2008; Ruff and Fehr, 2014), affective meaning (VMPFC: Roy et al., 2012; Delgado et al., 2016) and personal or biological relevance (amygdalae: Phelps and LeDoux, 2005; Adolphs, 2010). Moreover, all three regions have previously been implicated in positive evaluations of high-status individuals (Singer et al., 2004; Zink et al., 2008; Freeman et al., 2009; Cloutier et al., 2012; Cloutier and Gyurovski, 2014). Notably, this expected pattern of status-based evaluation was only present at low-EMS levels. At high-EMS levels, neural responses to status were reversed, with stronger activity for low-status compared with high-status targets. This finding implies that high-EMS levels may diminish the potential value or salience of high status during person evaluation. The finding may also suggest an aversion toward high-status individuals among high-EMS participants. The underlying mechanism of this EMS-related shift on status requires further study. Nonetheless, in the absence of any observed relationships between race and EMS, this pattern of findings is not easily explained by an EMS-related heightening of race salience (Richeson and Trawalter, 2008; Bean et al., 2012) at the expense of non-racial attributes such as status. More consistent with the literature and overall set of findings is the possibility that high-EMS individuals were indeed shifting their focus to non-racial attributes (viz., status) as they formed impressions of targets (Norton et al., 2006; Apfelbaum et al., 2008). However, this shift should not be characterized as an accentuation of the usual high-status–positive association (Varnum, 2013). One possibility is that the externally motivated shift to status-based evaluation to avoid race may have diminished the ordinarily high-motivational value assigned to high status (cf. Deci et al., 1999). Another possibility is that race-related EMS may generalize to other domains such as status. This potentially interesting question of domain generalization awaits further research.

Race effects

Although an extensive network of brain regions has been implicated in processing race (Kubota et al., 2012), this study did not observe differences as a function of perceived race. Perhaps more surprisingly, perceiver EMS did not reliably modulate the perception of race. We speculate that the presence of status may explain the absence of sensitivity to race (cf. Kurzban et al., 2001). Alternatively, the emphasis placed on status during the initial status training procedure with silhouettes may have encouraged participants to focus on status even after the introduction of real faces varying in race. We note that the neural representation of race is frequently sensitive to context. Early work on the neural correlates of race paid considerable attention to the role of the amygdala (Phelps et al., 2000); however, subsequent work has found that race-based activity in this region is sensitive to a number of factors. For example, when White perceivers are given sufficient time (Cunningham et al., 2004), specific instructions to form impressions of Black targets (Wheeler and Fiske, 2005; Li et al., 2016) or additional attributes (Van Bavel et al., 2008; Li et al., 2016), race-based amygdala activity tends not to be observed (Chekroud et al., 2014).

In this study, meaningful effects of EMS were observed in a relatively high-IMS sample. Notably, distinct behavioral profiles are associated with a perceiver’s combination of EMS and IMS (Butz and Plant, 2009). High-IMS individuals who are low in EMS are generally effective at regulating prejudice (Devine et al., 2002; Li et al., 2016). Individuals high in IMS and EMS are determined to control their prejudice. However, their preoccupation with appearing prejudiced makes them less effective in this endeavor (Amodio et al., 2008), perhaps especially when effortful control is difficult (e.g. under acute stress). Future work sampling a wider range of IMS is needed to better understand the role of EMS on neural evaluation in low-IMS perceivers.

Finally, although this study assessed cortisol levels in participants (Supplementary Material S2), the limited range of cortisol reactivity that was obtained precluded the opportunity to clearly investigate the role of stress on impression formation. Concerns about appearing prejudiced are known to be associated with increased physiological stress reactivity as measured by cortisol (Bijleveld et al., 2012; Trawalter et al., 2012). Although stress has been shown to impair cognitive processes relevant to intergroup decision-making (Arnsten, 2009), it remains unknown how stress and motivation interact to influence person evaluation. Additionally, it is unclear how acute increases in cortisol levels (i.e. stress) stemming from concerns about the social consequences of appearing prejudiced would interact with known status-related changes in cortisol (among other hormones: Carré et al., 2013; Pornpattananangkul et al., 2014; Hamilton et al., 2015; Mehta and Prasad, 2015; Ligneul and Dreher, 2017). These important questions lie at the heart of the relatively new but growing field of social neuroendocrinology (for reviews, see McCall and Singer, 2012; Hamilton et al., 2015).

Conclusion

At present, there are few empirical studies examining the intersections of race and status in person evaluation (but see Jussim et al., 1987; Kirby, 1999; Moore-Berg et al., 2017). Addressing this gap in social neuroscience, the present research revealed that White perceivers’ EMS altered neural processing of a non-racial attribute (viz., status) during person evaluation, independently of race. Specifically, results highlight that EMS may attenuate positive evaluations and/or the salience of high status. Future work is needed to characterize how regions associated with reward/salience (NAcc), person evaluation (VMPFC) and threat/relevance (amygdala) interface with the greater prejudice control network (Amodio, 2014) as a function of perceiver motivation and stress levels.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Tiara Starks and Jaelyn Peiso for assisting with recruitment and data collection. They also thank the MRI Research Center at the University of Chicago.

Funding

The center’s 3T scanner was supported by NIH grant 1S10OD018448-01.

Supplementary data

Supplementary data are available at SCAN online.

Conflict of interest. None declared.

References

(

).

What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition?

Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

, , –.

(

).

Intergroup anxiety effects on the control of racial stereotypes: a psychoneuroendocrine analysis

.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

, (), –.

(

).

The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping

.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

, (), –.

, , (

).

Individual differences in the regulation of intergroup bias: the role of conflict monitoring and neural signals for control

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , (

).

Individual differences in the activation and control of affective race bias as assessed by startle eyeblink response and self-report

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , (

).

Alternative mechanisms for regulating racial responses according to internal vs external cues

.

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

, (), –.

, , , , (

).

Modeling geometric deformations in EPI time series

. , (), –.

, , (

).

Seeing race and seeming racist? Evaluating strategic colorblindness in social interaction

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

(

).

Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function

.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

, (), –.

, (

).

Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions

. , (), –.

, , , (

).

Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4

.

Journal of Statistical Software

, (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Prejudice concerns and race-based attentional bias: new evidence from eyetracking

.

Social Psychological and Personality Science

, (), –.

, , (

).

To be liked versus respected: divergent goals in interracial interactions

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, (

).

Affective neuroscience of pleasure: reward in humans and animals

. , (), –.

, , , (

).

The cortisol response to anticipated intergroup interactions predicts self-reported prejudice

. , (),

, , , , (

).

Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatized others

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, (

).

Social class and race: burdens but also some benefits of chronic low rank

. , (), –.

, , (

).

Implicit race bias decreases the similarity of neural representations of black and white faces

. , (), –.

, (

).

Prejudice control and interracial relations: the role of motivation to respond without prejudice

. , (), –.

, , , , (

).

Changes in testosterone mediate the effect of winning on subsequent aggressive behaviour

. , (), –.

, , , (

).

A review of neuroimaging studies of race-related prejudice: does amygdala response reflect threat?

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

, (). doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00179.

, , , (

).

The neural substrates of person perception: spontaneous use of financial and moral status knowledge

. , (), –.

, , , , (

). Neuroimaging investigations of social status and social hierarchies. In: , , editors.

Neuroimaging Personality, Social Cognition, and Character

, –, : .

, (

).

Ventral medial prefrontal cortex and person evaluation: forming impressions of others varying in financial and moral status

. , , –.

, , , (

).

Are attractive people rewarding? Sex differences in the neural substrates of facial attractiveness

.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

, (), –.

, , (

).

The impact of childhood experience on amygdala response to perceptually familiar Black and White faces

.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

, (), –.

, , , (

).

Person perception and autonomic nervous system response: the costs and benefits of possessing a high social status

. , (), –.

, (

).

Valence and salience contribute to nucleus accumbens activation

. , (), –.

, , , , (

).

Dangerous enough: moderating racial bias with contextual threat cues

.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces

. , (), –.

(

).

Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI

. , (), –.

, , (

).

Monkeys pay per view: adaptive valuation of social images by rhesus macaques

. , (), –.

, , (

).

A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation

. , (), –.

, , , (

).

Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex

. , (), –.

, , , et al.  (

).

Viewpoints: dialogues on the functional role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex

. , (), –.

, , (

).

Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game

. , (), –.

, , (

). Exploring the interpersonal dynamics of intergroup contact. In: , , editors.

Handbook of Motivation and Cognition, Volume 3: The Interpersonal Context

, –, : .

, , , , (

).

The regulation of explicit and implicit race bias: the role of motivations to respond without prejudice

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , , (

).

Reducing susceptibility artifacts in fMRI using volume-selective z-shim compensation

.

Magnetic Resonance in Medicine

, (), –.

, , (

).

Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates

.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

, (), –.

(

). Interpersonal stratification: status, power, and subordination. In: , , , editors.

Handbook of Social Psychology

, –, : .

, , , (

).

Status, power, and intergroup relations: the personal is the societal

.

Current Opinion in Psychology

, , –.

, (

).

Three ways in which midline regions contribute to self-evaluation

.

Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

, ,

, , , , (

).

Looking the part: social status cues shape race perception

. , (),

, , , (

).

Culture shapes a mesolimbic response to signals of dominance and subordination that associates with behavior

. , (), –.

, , , , (

).

Stochastic designs in event-related fMRI

. , (), –.

, (

). The aversive form of racism. In: , , editors.

Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism

, –, : .

, , (

).

Evaluative vs. trait representation in intergroup social judgments: distinct roles of anterior temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex

. , (), –.

, , , , (

).

Social neuroendocrinology of status: a review and future directions

.

Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology

, (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs ingroup face stimuli

. , (), –.

, (

).

Effects of external and internal motivation to control prejudice on implicit prejudice: the mediating role of efforts to control prejudiced responses

.

Basic and Applied Social Psychology

, (), –.

(

).

Compositions in Black and White: determinants of interaction in interracial dyads

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , et al.  (

).

Toward a comprehensive understanding of executive cognitive function in implicit racial bias

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , (

).

The nature of stereotypes: a comparison and integration of three theories

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

(

).

Income source and race effects on new-neighbor evaluations

.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , (

).

Nucleus accumbens activation mediates the infuence of reward cues on financial risk taking

. , (), –.

, , (

).

Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization

.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

, (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Stereotypes as dominant responses: on the “social facilitation” of prejudice in anticipated public contexts

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, (

).

Racial assumptions color the mental representation of social class

. , ,

, , , (

).

The impact of motivation on race-based impression formation

. , , –.

, (

). Social dominance representations in the human brain. In: , , editors. , –,

Amsterdam, The Netherlands

: .

, , , , (

).

Subjective socioeconomic status predicts human ventral striatal responses to social status information

. , (), –.

, , (

).

How social status shapes person perception and evaluation: a social neuroscience perspective

.

Perspectives on Psychological Science

, (), –.

, (

).

The animal and human neuroendocrinology of social cognition, motivation and behavior

. , (), –.

, (

).

The dual-hormone hypothesis: a brief review and future research agenda

.

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences

, , –.

, , , , (

).

Threatened by the unexpected: physiological responses during social interactions with expectancy-violating partners

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , (

).

Challenge and threat during social interactions with White and Black men

.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

, (), –.

, , (

).

Encoding-specific effects of social cognition on the neural correlates of subsequent memory

. , (), –.

, , (

).

Forming impressions of people versus inanimate objects: social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex

. , (), –.

, , (

).

Quick to the draw: how suspect race and socioeconomic status influences shooting decisions

.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology

. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12454.

, , , , (

).

Color blindness and interracial interaction: playing the political correctness game

. , (), –.

, (

). Measures of prejudice. In: , editor.

Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination

, 2nd edn, –, : .

, , (

).

Intergroup contact facilitates physiological recovery following stressful intergroup interactions

.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , (

).

With a little help from my cross-group friend: reducing anxiety in intergroup contexts through cross-group friendship

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , (

).

Effects of direct and indirect cross-group friendships on judgments of Catholics and Protestants in Northern Ireland: the mediating role of an anxiety-reduction mechanism

.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

, (), –.

, (

).

How social status shapes race

.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

, (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study

. , (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: a trial-related fMRI study

. , (), –.

, (

).

Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: from animal models to human behavior

. , (), –.

, , , et al.  (

).

Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation

.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

, (), –.

, (

).

Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, (

).

The antecedents and implications of interracial anxiety

.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

, (), –.

, , (

). Neural basis of social status hierarchy. In: , , , editors.

The Psychology of Social Status

, –, : .

. ().

R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing

. :

R Foundation for Statistical Computing

. Available: https://www.r-project.org/ (Accessed October 4, 2017).

, , , et al.  (

).

An fMRI investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function

. , (), –.

, (

).

When prejudice does not pay: effects of interracial contact on executive function

. , (), –.

, (

).

Toward a social psychology of race and race relations for the twenty-first century

.

Annual Review of Psychology

, (), –.

, , , (

).

Eye-gaze direction modulates race-related amygdala activity

.

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations

, (), –.

, (

).

The threat of appearing prejudiced and race-based attentional biases

. , (), –.

, , , , , (

).

The effects of skin tone on race-related amygdala activity: an fMRI investigation

.

Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

, (), –.

, , (

).

Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning

.

Trends in Cognitive Sciences

, (), –.

, (

).

The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making

.

Nature Reviews Neuroscience

, (), –.

, (

). Putting race in context: socioeconomic status predicts racial fluidity. In: , , editors.

Facing Social Class: How Societal Rank Influences Interaction

, –, : .

, , (

).

Social status gates social attention in monkeys

. , (), –.

, , , , (

).

Brain reponses to the acquired moral status of faces

. , (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Slice-timing effects and their correction in functional MRI

. , (), –.

, (

).

Evaluative race–class stereotypes by race and perceived class of subjects

.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , et al.  (

).

Race and reputation: perceived racial group trustworthiness influences the neural correlates of trust decisions

.

Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences

, (), –.

, (

).

Promote up, ingratiate down: status comparisons drive warmth-competence tradeoffs in impression management

.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

, , –.

, , , (

).

Concerns about appearing prejudiced get under the skin: stress responses to interracial contact in the moment and across time

.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , (

).

The neural substrates of in-group bias: a functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation

. , (), –.

(

).

What are lay theories of social class?

, (),

, (

).

Complicating race: the relationship between prejudice, race, and social class categorizations

.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

, (), –.

, (

).

Controlling racial prejudice: social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation

. , (), –.

(

).

Motivational influences on compliance with and consequences of instructions to suppress stereotypes

.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

, (), –.

, , , , , (

).

Know your place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans

. , (), –.

© The Author (2017). Published by Oxford University Press.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-Commercial License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. For commercial re-use, please contact [email protected]

How can you increase intrinsic motivation?

Increased intrinsic motivation can be encouraged by building environments that promote:.
Autonomy – people need to believe they have choices. ... .
Relatedness – people need to deepen their connections with others. ... .
Competence – people need to have the right skills and the opportunity to show them..

What are 3 ways to overcome prejudice?

Education and Re-education. Unsurprisingly, education has long been a key area of interest for scholars in all disciplines who have looked at 'what works' to reduce prejudice. ... .
Short-term 'diversity training' courses. ... .
Media-based interventions..

What is the best way to overcome prejudice?

You can ask people to tone it down. You can discuss the issue or make yourself heard in another way. You can let people know that you are not okay with offensive or insulting prejudices - whether they affect others or yourself.

What are the 3 types of intrinsic motivation?

Three types of intrinsic motivation.
Autonomy - We are much more likely to engage with activities we have chosen to do..
Mastery - We are intrinsically motivated to get better and better at things that matter to us..
Purpose - We are motivated when our actions are in service of something larger than ourselves..